Introduction
Students generally experience a wide range of emotions during learning activities in classroom (Pekrun et al., 2002), which can, in turn, significantly affect their learning processes (Dörnyei, 2014). The crucial importance of emotions has been generally emphasized in learning (Pekrun, 2006; Weiner, 2010) to the extent that the teaching activity and the classroom environment have been considered as an ‘emotional practice’ (Hargreaves, 2001) and an ‘emotional place’ (Pekrun & Linnenbrink-Garcia, 2014), respectively.
Similar to learning other subjects (e.g., mathematics and science), the language learning process and language classes are replete with a wide variety of emotions including anxiety, shame, enjoyment, and pride, etc. (Bown & White, 2010; Shao et al., 2020; Swain, 2013). Foreign/Second language (L2) learning is best considered as not only a cognitive/rational process but also a social and ‘emotionally-charged’ one (Richards, 2022). As key dynamic processes in L2 learning, emotions are “affective experiences that are tied directly to language learning activities and resulting learning outcomes” (Shao et al., 2019, p. 2) and can affect learners’ attention and cognitive processes, interest, and engagement in foreign language learning (Shao et al., 2019). Therefore, it is highly crucial to take into account their role in L2 learning.
Research on emotions in foreign language learning
Research on factors affecting how students learn has captured the importance of both cognitive and emotional factors. However, most studies have generally focused on cognitive aspects while failing to pay due attention to the essential role of emotions in L2 learning (Dewaele, 2015; Richards, 2022; Shao et al., 2019; White, 2018). Motivated by the extensive research addressing emotions in educational psychology, a growing body of studies has recently addressed emotions in L2 learning over the past decade (Gkonou et al., 2020; Pekrun et al., 2019). However, the extant research on L2 emotions has mainly addressed negative emotions such as anxiety (see Cheng, 2017; Teimouri et al., 2019; Zhang, 2019) as well as shame and guilt (Teimouri, 2018) rather than positive emotions.
Over the past few years, following the ‘emotional turn’ in applied linguistics (Benesch, 2013) as well as the application of positive psychology in L2 research (see Budzińska & Majchrzak, 2021; Derakhshan, 2022; Dewaele et al., 2018; Kushkiev, 2019; Oladrostam & Rezaee, 2019; Rezaee & Oladrostam, 2018), an unprecedented number of studies have focused on the significant and facilitative role of positive emotions in L2 learning (e.g., Reilly, 2021; Shao et al., 2020; Zhang & Tsung, 2021). For example, several studies have documented the important effect of positive emotions on learners’ motivation (Dörnyei, 2014), engagement (Arnold, 2009; Sadoughi & Hejazi, 2021), grit (Hejazi & Sadoughi, 2023), willingness to communicate (Sadoughi & Hejazi, 2023a), interest and thinking (Fredrickson & Losada, 2005), creativity, performance, and social cohesion (Shao et al., 2019). In fact, a growing body of research has addressed the undeniable role of positive emotions in L2 learning.
Positive emotions in foreign language learning
Positive emotions are considered as one of the three main elements of positive psychology (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Based on broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 2001), positive emotions have an enormous potential to help EFL learners broaden their perspectives to specifically devote their cognitive resources to foreign language learning processes (Shao et al., 2019). Additionally, recent research has showed that positive emotions have a positive relationship with working memory (Figueira et al., 2018), motivation (Pekrun, 2014), and self-regulated learning (Alexander et al., 2018). Furthermore, fostering positive emotions in L2 classes can help build “an affectively positive environment [which can] put the brain in the optimal state for learning” (Arnold, 2009, p. 146). In fact, positive emotions enhance students’ interest and thinking and enable them to concentrate their attention on learning activities by promoting their awareness of the foreign language input and engagement in learning tasks (Dewaele et al., 2016; White, 2018). Additionally, they can increase learners’ willingness to communicate and risk-taking (Dewaele et al., 2018). Furthermore, they help learners maintain their interest and sustain their effort in the lengthy process of learning a foreign language (Hejazi & Sadoughi, 2023). Hence, effective measures should be taken to not only decrease the debilitating effects of learners’ negative emotions but also enhance their positive emotions (Gregersen, 2013), which can in turn maximize their involvement and engagement in foreign language classes (Richards, 2022).
Measures of L2 positive emotions
A growing body of studies has recently focused on measuring positive emotions relating to foreign language learning. However, most of these studies have mainly addressed ‘enjoyment’ by developing and/or validating measurement scales (see Botes et al., 2021, 2022; Dewaele & MacIntyre, 2016; Jiang & Dewaele, 2019; Jin & Zhang, 2018; Li et al., 2018). The review of literature indicates that while foreign language enjoyment has received growing attention (Elahi Shirvan et al., 2021; Dewaele, 2022), there has been a lesser research focus on other positive emotions such as ‘hope’ and ‘pride’.
One comprehensive, domain-general, and theory-grounded scale to assess positive and negative achievement emotions is Achievement Emotions Questionnaire (AEQ) which was originally developed by Pekrun et al. (2011). AEQ is one of the most widely used measures in education and educational psychology to investigate learners’ achievement emotions. Deeply rooted in control-value theory (Pekrun, 2006) and informed by the social-cognitive perspective, AEQ classifies different emotions (enjoyment, hope, pride, relief, anger, anxiety, hopelessness, shame, and boredom) based on three main dimensions, namely, valence (positive or negative), activation level (activating or deactivating), and object focus (activity or outcome) in learning, classroom, and test situations, and at different time intervals corresponding to each situation (i.e., before, during, and after) (Pekrun et al., 2011). It should be noted that several studies have showed that there are meaningful relationships between emotions measured by AEQ and external variables such as achievement and attributions of success and failure (Frenzel et al., 2007) as well as psychobiological personality dimensions (Moreira et al., 2019).
Several attempts have been made to validate the AEQ in different domains and contexts (e.g., Bhansali & Sharma, 2020; Fierro-Suero et al., 2020; Govaerts & Grégoire, 2008; Lichtenfeld et al., 2012; Moreira et al., 2019; Peixoto et al., 2015). These studies have generally confirmed the psychometric proprieties of AEQ in different populations. In a recent study, Bieleke et al. (2021) introduced the short form of AEQ and reported its high validity and reliability. The short version includes 96 items measuring eight different emotions relating to classroom, learning, and test with four items per scale. Similar to the original questionnaire, the short version is correlated with external measures, i.e., antecedents (e.g., perceived academic control, self-efficacy, and task value) and consequences of academic emotions (e.g., motivation, learning strategies, and self-regulation).
Although several L2 studies have used AEQ to measure emotions (e.g., Reilly & Rosas, 2019; Tan, 2017; Yükselir & Harputlu, 2014), only a few studies have attempted to examine the factor structure of this questionnaire to measure emotions relating to foreign language learning and classroom in general and positive emotions in particular. In a recent study, Davari et al. (2020) examined the factor structure of only the classroom emotions sub-scales among Iranianforeign language learners through Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA). Their findings indicated that the classroom sub-scale for positive and negative emotions had a good model fit, and its factor structure was invariant across gender (i.e., male and female) and learning context (language institutes and university). This validation study did not consider other sub-scales of AEQ, i.e., those relating to learning and test situations. To the best of our knowledge, no systematic study has specifically focused on all positive emotions of EFL learners in learning and classroom settings based on AEQ.
It is argued that the validity of psychometric measures should be examined for further use in different contexts (Li et al., 2018) and domains (Butz et al., 2016; Goetz et al., 2007; Götz et al. 2010; Shao et al., 2019). Accordingly, in response to Shao et al.’s (2019) call for further extensive research on achievement emotions in L2 and considering the utmost importance and the excellent potential of positive emotions in L2 learning (MacIntyre et al., 2019; Shao et al., 2020), the present study aims to fill in this research gap by focusing on positive emotions in foreign language learning and classroom. More specifically, this study is an attempt to prepare a psychometrically valid and reliable measure called Foreign Language Positive Emotions Questionnaire (FL-PEQ) based on AEQ. It should be noted that AEQ is a rather long measure with a large number of items, which could make it difficult and time-consuming to administer in empirical studies. Therefore, following Dörnyei and Taguchi’s (2010) suggestions to ensure an appropriate length of questionnaires for efficient administration and avoid respondents’ fatigue, the present study aims to prepare a shortversion for assessing positive emotions in foreign language classroom and learning.
Method
Participants and procedure
A total of 442 Iranian EFL university students in Tehran (the capital city of Iran) were randomly chosen via multi-stage cluster sampling from six leading state universities in winter 2019. At the time of data collection, the students (50.5% males & 49.5% females; mean age=21.1 years) were taking a compulsory general English course offered by English language departments in their universities. They were all native speakers of Persian, had learned English in secondary school, and were at an intermediate proficiency level. A total of 450 questionnaires adapted from the AEQ were distributed among the participants, and the return rate was 99%. In the questionnaires, the participants first provided their demographic features (age & gender) and then completed the adapted version of the AEQ sub-scales measuring positive emotions (enjoyment, hope, pride) relating to foreign language learning and classroom. They had adequate time to respond to the questionnaire items.
To recruit the sample, the offices of vice deans of educational affairs in the selected universities were first contacted to obtain their permission. To this end, we explained the aims of the study for them and received their permission for data collection. To ensure the students’ maximum understanding, the questionnaire was administered in Persian, the students’ native language. The participants were assured about the anonymity and confidentiality of the collected data. Additionally, they were informed that participation in this study was entirely voluntary, and they could withdraw at any stage. Furthermore, their informed consent was obtained. The exclusion criteria were refusal to give informed consent, unwillingness to continue the study, and incomplete questionnaires.
Instrument
AEQ (Pekrun et al., 2011) sub-scales for positive emotions relating to learning (enjoyment: 10 items; hope: 8 items; pride: 9 items) and classroom (enjoyment: 10 items; hope: 6 items; pride: 6 items) were adapted by inserting the word ‘English’ in the items and applying some necessary modifications. The items were first translated from English to Persian by two experts and then back-translated to English by two other translators. The inconsistencies and ambiguities were resolved by a panel of experts in translation, educational psychology, and English language teaching. Furthermore, six students were asked to check the clarity of the items. Accordingly, some minor modifications were applied based on their feedback. The questionnaire had six sub-scales measuring different positive emotions (i.e., enjoyment, hope, and pride) relating to foreign language learning and classroom. The items were presented on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from ‘strongly disagree’ (1) to ‘strongly agree’ (5).
Data analysis
Preliminary analysis
Prior to the main analysis, the data were checked for missing data as well as univariate and multivariate normality. First, six questionnaires with missing values on more than five items were excluded. Next, the percentage of missing data per each variable was computed as 0-1.1%. The results of Little’s test showed that the data were missing completely at random (MCAR) (χ2 (1116)=915.30, p=1.00). Therefore, Multiple Imputation using the EM algorithm was used to replace the isolated missing values in the data set. Then, the univariate normality was assessed through standardized scores, and multivariate normality was evaluated by Mahalanobis Distance. Consequently, three multivariate outliers were removed. The values of skewness (-.004 to .47) and kurtosis (-.15 to -.98) were within the recommended ranges for CFA with maximum likelihood estimation (West et al., 1995), which suggested that all variables met the normality assumption.
Constructing the short version
To construct a short version of the questionnaire for measuring positive emotions in foreign language learning and classroom, five items with the highest factor loadings in each of the six sub-scales were retained, which resulted in 30 items. Confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs) and maximum likelihood estimation were performed to assess the factor structure of the scales. Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and Standardized Root Mean Squared Residual (SRMR) were used to analyze the model fit.
Statistical procedure
After the preliminary analysis, two CFAs were first run to check the construct validity of ‘the adapted sub-scales of the AEQ measuring positive emotions relating to learning and classroom’ as well as ‘the shortened version prepared in this study (FL-PEQ)’. Next, the construct validity of the one-, two-, and six-factor models of the FL-PEQ were examined to evaluate their internal validity. In addition, the convergent validity of the FL-PEQ was assessed by Average Variance Extracted (AVE) and composite reliability. Additionally, Fornell and Larker criterion and heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) ratio were checked to evaluate the discriminant validity. Furthermore, the reliability of the FL-PEQ was investigated by part-whole corrected item-total correlations.
Results
CFA was run in Amos software (Arbuckle, 2019) to evaluate the factor structure of the adapted sub-scales measuring positive emotions relating to learning and classroom, which indicated a good model fit (Table 1). After retaining five items with the highest factor loadings in each sub-scale, another CFA was run to examine the model fit of the short version. As Table 1 shows, the short version has a better model fit in comparison to the original version.
Table 1: CFA results comparing the original and short versions of FL-PEQ
Table 2 shows the retained 30 items with the highest factor loadings as well as their mean, standard deviation, skewness, and kurtosis. In what follows, the validity and reliability of the FL-PEQ are investigated.
Table 2: Descriptive statistics relating to the items of the FL-PEQ
Internal validity
CFA was carried out to assess the internal validity of the FL-PEQ. The construct validity of three models, namely, one-factor, two-factor, and six-factor models, was investigated competitively. Model A (one-factor model) assumed that the whole scale was based on only one factor while Model B (two-factor model) distinguished between the latent factors, namely, classroom emotions and learning emotions. In addition, in Model C (six-factor model), six latent primary factors for each positive emotion (enjoyment, hope, and pride) and two secondary latent emotion factors (i.e., classroom emotions and learning emotions) were assumed. Based on Table 3, neither Model A nor Model B has a good fit to the data. However, the hypothesized hierarchical Model C has a good fit with the empirical data. To sum up, confirmatory factor analyses corroborated the six-factor structure by distinguishing between different positive emotions in classroom and learning situations.
Table 3: Summary of model fit statistics for the one-factor, two-factor, and six-factor models
In addition, as another criterion for assessing internal validity, inter-correlations between different positive emotions across the two situations (i.e., learning and classroom) indicated that all three emotions were correlated. For example, students experiencing more enjoyment in foreign language classroom reported higher levels of enjoyment in foreign language learning. To conclude, the inter-correlations among the three emotions in the two situations confirmed the internal validity of the FL-PEQ.
Convergent validity
The convergent validity of the FL-PEQ was examined to confirm the validity of each subscale. Based on Table 4, the model in each subscale has a good fit with the data, which indicates that the collected data were suitable for convergent validity analysis.
Table 4: Convergent validity, reliability, and model fit of the FL-PEQ
Discriminant validity
Discriminant validity was checked with two widely used methods, namely, Fornell and Larker criterion and heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) ratio, which were performed in SmartPLS software (version 3.2.9) (Ringle et al., 2015). To examine the discriminant validity using Fornel and Larcker (1981), the square root of each AVE in the diagonal was compared with the correlation coefficients (off-diagonal) for each construct in the relevant rows and columns below the diagonal. AVE square roots for each construct which are greater than the correlation between the constructs indicate discriminant validity between the constructs. As shown in Table 5, the correlations between the constructs are less than the square root of the AVE of each construct.
Table 5: Fornell-Larker criterion and HTMT ratios with 95% confidence interval
Discriminant validity was also evaluated using HTMT based on Multitrait-Multimethod (MTMM) matrix. In this study, HTMT was applied as both a criterion and a statistical test. HTMT results lower than .85 indicate discriminant validity (Kline, 2011). In addition, bootstrap procedure was used with a confidence interval to check whether the confidence interval included one. Confidence intervals including the value one indicate the lack of discriminant validity while those not including the value one are indicative of the construct distinctiveness. Based on Table 5, the FL-PEQ has discriminant validity since all HTMT ratios are below .85 and the confidence intervals (shown in parentheses) do not contain the value one.
Reliability
The Cronbach alpha coefficients of different FL-PEQ sub-scales ranged from .83 to .93. Additionally, part-whole corrected item-total correlations for all items were higher than .5 (see Table 4), which is considered very good (Bortz & Döring, 2006). Hence, the FL-PEQ scales have sufficient reliability.
Measurement invariance of the FL-PEQ across gender
The measurement equivalence of the six-factor model was examined across gender to make sure that it could be used to measure positive emotions of both male and female students. Before analyzing measurement invariance, this model was checked to examine whether it showed a reasonably good fit to the empirical data in male and female groups separately (see Table 6). Model fit indices such as CFI, TLI, RMSEA, and SRMR were satisfactory for both gender groups. The configural invariance model, in which no equality constraints were imposed, had an adequate fit to the data. Next, the metric invariance was checked for the two groups, and all model fit indices were acceptable. The invariance of the models was checked following Cheung and Rensvold’s (2002) recommendation based on which ΔCFIs less than .01 indicate the model invariance. The ΔCFI between the configural and metric models was below .01, which showed that the hypothesis of metric invariance could be retained. Then, scalar model invariance, in which the intercepts and factor loadings were constrained to be equal across groups, was also examined. The ΔCFI between the scalar and metric invariance models was less than .01, which indicated that scalar invariance could be confirmed. In addition, the differences in Macdonald non-centrality (ΔMC NCI) between the models were .0031 and .0008 for metric and scalar models, respectively, which are less than the recommended value of .02 by McDonald and Marsh (1990). Additionally, gamma hats were higher than .90. Furthermore, the differences in gamma hats were .00056 and .00016 for metric and scalar models, respectively, which are less than the recommended value of .001 (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002). Therefore, all indices support configural, metric, and scalar invariance of the six-factor model of the FL-PEQ across gender (see Table 6).
Table 6: Goodness-of-fit indices of measurement invariance across gender for FL-PEQ six-factor model
Gender differences of the FL-PEQ
Given the measurement invariance of the FL-PEQ across gender, differences between the means of the sub-scales could be examined. The MANOVA results revealed that female students had significantly higher scores on all the six sub-scales: classroom related enjoyment (M±SDmales=14.12±3.88; M±SDfemales=16.15±3.44; F=33.70; p< .001; ƞ2=.071), classroom-related hope (M±SDmales=14.17±4.58; M±SDfemales=15.51±4.29; F=10; p=.002; η2=.022), classroom-related pride (M±SDmales=15.85±4.20; M±SDfemales=16.73±3.85; F=5.34; p=.021; ƞ2=.012), learning-related enjoyment (M±SDmales=16.98±4.26; M±SDfemales=18.27±3.62; F=11.70; p=.001; ƞ2=.026), learning-related hope (M±SDmales=15.64±4.3; M±SDfemales=16.37±3.93; F=5.35; p=.021; ƞ2=.012), and learning-related pride (M±SDmales=15.21±4.87; M±SDfemales=16.53±4.7; F=8.31; p=.004; ƞ2=.019).
Discussion and Conclusion
The present study aimed to prepare a valid and reliable scale to measure positive emotions in foreign language learningand classroom situations. To this end, the items (n=49) relating to positive emotions were adopted from AEQ as a well-known, domain-general measure with good psychometric qualities. Then, they were adapted for the purpose of the current study and administered among Iranian EFL university students. The results of data analysis indicated that this measure is psychometrically valid and reliable.
In order to prepare a short measure for efficient administration and avoid respondents’ fatigue, it was decided to shorten the measure by choosing five items with the highest factor loadings from each of the six sub-scales, which resulted in a total of 30 items. Consistent with Pekrun et al.’s (2011) landmark study on undergraduate students, a six-factor model of this short version was corroborated, which indicated that the FL-PEQ as a shortened measure could adequately differentiate between enjoyment, hope, and pride across two important foreign language settings, namely, learning and classroom. In addition, the convergent validity and internal validity of the FL-PEQ were confirmed.
Considering the importance of positive emotions in foreign language learning and classroom, the FL-PEQ could be used as a valid and reliable measure to assess positive emotions. More specifically, given the undeniably important effect of positive emotions on students’ motivation, engagement, grit, creativity, performance, etc. and the essential role of teachers in this regard (Hejazi & Sadoughi, 2023; Sadoughi & Hejazi, 2021; Sadoughi & Hejazi, 2022, 2023b), it is vital that EFL teachers as well as materials developers use FL-PEQ to evaluate learners’ positive emotions towards classroom and learning and take significant measures to promote them. Accordingly, this will help create a balance between attention to emotional as well as cognitive aspects of learning a foreign language.
Implications, limitations, and suggestions for further research
Positive emotions have an excellent potential in promoting key outcomes in education (e.g., engagement and grit) as well as broadening learners’ perspectives and enhancing the development of necessary cognitive resources for foreign language learning. Therefore, given the importance of positive emotions in education in general (Pekrun, 2006; Weiner, 2010) and L2 learning in particular (Shao et al., 2019), the FL-PEQ could be used as a reliable and valid measure to assess EFL learners’ positive emotions and take effective measures to promote them accordingly. Additionally, researchers can use this short scale as a research tool to measure EFL learners’ positive emotions and investigate theireffects on other variables as well as their possible associations.
The present study had some limitations. The data were collected using only self-report questionnaires, which could have some problems such as superficiality, social desirability, and halo effect (Dörnyei & Taguchi, 2010). Therefore, it is suggested that future research use qualitative data collection techniques such as stimulated recall and interview to explore EFL learners’ positive emotions more deeply.
Care should be taken in using the FL-PEQ in other contexts since this measure was validated in Iran, as an expanding circle country with its own particular political, educational, socio-economic, and cultural characteristics, where English is used as a foreign language (Zare-ee & Hejazi, 2017, 2018). Therefore, future studies could validate this measure in English as a Second Language (ESL) as well as other EFL contexts. Last but not the least, it is suggested that further studies be conducted to examine the psychometric properties of the FL-PEQ across different language skills (i.e., speaking, writing, listening, and reading) and settings (e.g., private language institutes and schools).
References
Arbuckle, J. L. (2019). Amos (Version 26.0) [Computer Program]. IBM SPSS
Arnold, J. (2009). Affect in L2 learning and teaching. Elia: Estudios de lingüística inglesa aplicada, 9, 145-151. https://revistas.uned.es/index.php/ELIA/article/view/18069/15230
Benesch, S. (2013). Considering emotions in critical English language teaching: Theories and praxis. Routledge.
Bhansali, A., & Sharma, M. D. (2020). The achievement emotions questionnaire: Validation and implementation for undergraduate physics practicals. International Journal of Innovation in Science and Mathematics Education, 27(9), 34-46. https://doi.org/10.30722/IJISME.27.09.003
Bieleke, M., Gogol, K., Goetz, T., Daniels, L., & Pekrun, R. (2021). The AEQ-S: A short version of the Achievement Emotions Questionnaire. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 65, 1-15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101940
Bortz, J., & Döring, N. (2006). Forschungsmethoden und Evaluation für Human-und Sozialwissenschaftler [Research methods and evaluation for human and social scientists]. Springer.
Botes, E., Dewaele, J.-M., & Greiff, S. (2021). The development and validation of the short form of the Foreign Language Enjoyment Scale. The Modern Language Journal, 105(4), 858-876. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12741
Botes, E., Dewaele, J.-M., & Greiff, S. (2022). Taking stock: A meta-analysis of the effects of foreign language enjoyment. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 12(2), 205-232. https://doi.org/10.14746/ssllt.2022.12.2.3
Bown, J., & White, C. J. (2010). Affect in a self-regulatory framework for language learning. System, 38(3), 432-443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2010.03.016
Budzińska, K., & Majchrzak, O. (2021). Positive psychology in second and foreign language education. Springer.
Butz, N. T., Stupnisky, R. H., Pekrun, R., Jensen, J. L., & Harsell, D. M. (2016). The impact of emotions on student achievement in synchronous hybrid business and public administration programs: A longitudinal test of control‐value theory. Decision Sciences Journal of Innovative Education, 14(4), 441-474. https://doi.org/10.1111/dsji.12110
Cheng, Y-s.. (2017). Development and preliminary validation of four brief measures of L2 language skill-specific anxiety. System, 68, 15-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2017.06.009
Cheung, G. W., and Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(2), 233–255. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5
Davari, H., Karami, H., Nourzadeh, S., & Iranmehr, A. (2020). Examining the validity of the Achievement Emotions Questionnaire for measuring more emotions in the foreign language classroom. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 43(8), 701-714.https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2020.1766054
Derakhshan, A. (2022). Revisiting research on positive psychology in second and foreign language education: Trends and directions. Language Related Research, 13(5), 1-43. https://doi.org/10.52547/LRR.13.5.1
Dewaele, J.-M. (2015). On emotions in foreign language learning and use. The Language Teacher, 39(3), 13-15. https://doi.org/10.37546/JALTTLT39.3-3
Dewaele, J.-M. (2022). Enjoyment. In S. Li, P. Hiver, & M. Papi (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and individual differences (pp. 190-206). Routledge.
Dewaele, J. M., & MacIntyre, P. D. (2016). Foreign language enjoyment and foreign language classroom anxiety: The right and left feet of the language learner. In P. D. MacIntyre, T. Gregersen, & S. Mercer (Eds.), Positive psychology in SLA (pp. 215-236). Multilingual Matters.
Dewaele, J.-M., MacIntyre, P., Boudreau, C., & Dewaele, L. (2016). Do girls have all the fun? Anxiety and enjoyment in the foreign language classroom. Theory and practice of second language acquisition, 2(1), 41-63. https://journals.us.edu.pl/index.php/TAPSLA/article/view/3941
Dewaele, J.-M., Witney, J., Saito, K., & Dewaele, L. (2018). Foreign language enjoyment and anxiety: The effect of teacher and learner variables. Language Teaching Research, 22(6), 676-697. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168817692161
Dörnyei, Z. & Taguchi, T. (2010). Questionnaires in second language research: Construction, administration, and processing. Routledge.
Dörnyei, Z. (2014). The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in second language acquisition. Routledge.
Elahi Shirvan, M., Taherian, T., & Yazdanmehr, E. (2021). Foreign language enjoyment: a longitudinal confirmatory factor analysis–curve of factors model. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2021.1874392
Fierro-Suero, S., Almagro, B. J., & Sáenz-López, P. (2020). Validation of the Achievement Emotions Questionnaire for Physical Education (AEQ-PE). International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(12). https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17124560
Figueira, J. S. B., Pacheco, L. B., Lobo, I., Volchan, E., Pereira, M. G., de Oliveira, L., & David, I. A. (2018). “Keep that in mind!” The role of positive affect in working memory for maintaining goal-relevant information. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01228
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218-226. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218
Fredrickson, B. L., & Losada, M. F. (2005). Positive affect and the complex dynamics of human flourishing. American Psychologist, 60(7), 678-686. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.60.7.678
Frenzel, A. C., Thrash, T. M., Pekrun, R., & Goetz, T. (2007). Achievement emotions in Germany and China: A cross-cultural validation of the Academic Emotions Questionnaire—Mathematics. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(3), 302-309. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022107300276
Gkonou, C., Dewaele, J.-M., & King, J. (2020). The emotional rollercoaster of language teaching. Multilingual Matters.
Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., & Lüdtke, O. (2007). Between-and within-domain relations of students’ academic emotions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(4), 715- 733. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.4.715
Götz, T., Cronjaeger, H., Frenzel, A. C., Lüdtke, O., & Hall, N. C. (2010). Academic self-concept and emotion relations: Domain specificity and age effects. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35(1), 44-58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2009.10.001
Govaerts, S., & Grégoire, J. (2008). Development and construct validation of an academic emotions scale. International Journal of Testing, 8(1), 34-54. https://doi.org/10.1080/15305050701808649
Gregersen, T. (2013). Language learning vibes: What, why and how to capitalize for positive affect. In D. Gabryś-Barker & J. Bielska (Eds.), The affective dimension in second language acquisition (pp. 89-98). Multilingual Matters.
Hargreaves, A. (2001). Emotional geographies of teaching. Teachers College Record, 103(6), 1056-1080. https://doi.org/10.1111/0161-4681.00142
Hejazi, S. Y., & Sadoughi, M. (2023). How does teacher support contribute to learners’ grit? The role of learning enjoyment. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching, 17(3), 593-606. https://doi.org/10.1080/17501229.2022.2098961
Jiang, Y., & Dewaele, J.-M. (2019). How unique is the foreign language classroom enjoyment and anxiety of Chinese EFL learners? System, 82, 13-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2019.02.017
Jin, Y., & Zhang, L. J. (2018). The dimensions of foreign language classroom enjoyment and their effect on foreign language achievement. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 24(7), 948-962. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2018.1526253
Kline, R. (2011). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). Guilford.
Kushkiev, P. (2019). The role of positive emotions in second language acquisition: Some critical considerations. MEXTESOL Journal, 43(4). https://mextesol.net/journal/index.php?page=journal&id_article=14709
Li, C., Jiang, G., & Dewaele, J.-M. (2018). Understanding Chinese high school students’ foreign language enjoyment: Validation of the Chinese version of the Foreign Language Enjoyment scale. System, 76, 183-196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2018.06.004
Lichtenfeld, S., Pekrun, R., Stupnisky, R. H., Reiss, K., & Murayama, K. (2012). Measuring students’ emotions in the early years: The Achievement Emotions Questionnaire-Elementary School (AEQ-ES). Learning and Individual Differences, 22(2), 190-201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.04.00
MacIntyre, P. D., Gregersen, T., & Mercer, S. (2019). Setting an agenda for positive psychology in SLA: Theory, practice, and research. The Modern Language Journal, 103(1), 262-274. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12544
McDonald, R. P., & Marsh, H. W. (1990). Choosing a multivariate model: Noncentrality and goodness-of-fit. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 247–255. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.247
Moreira, P., Cunha, D., & Inman, R. A. (2019). Achievement Emotions Questionnaire-Mathematics (AEQ-M) in adolescents: Factorial structure, measurement invariance and convergent validity with personality. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(6), 750-762. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2018.1548349
Oladrostam, E., & Rezaee, A. (2019). Moving second language acquisition one step forward: Validating an inventory of positive psychology in language learning. MEXTESOL Journal, 43(4). https://www.mextesol.net/journal/index.php?page=journal&id_article=14794
Peixoto, F., Mata, L., Monteiro, V., Sanches, C., & Pekrun, R. (2015). The Achievement Emotions Questionnaire: Validation for pre-adolescent students. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12(4), 472-481. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2015.1040757
Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: Assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educational Psychology Review, 18(4), 315-341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-006-9029-9
Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Barchfeld, P., & Perry, R. P. (2011). Measuring emotions in students’ learning and performance: The Achievement Emotions Questionnaire (AEQ). Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36(1), 36-48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.002
Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. P. (2002). Positive emotions in education. In E. Frydenberg (Ed.), Beyond coping: Meeting goals, visions, and challenges (pp. 149-173). Oxford University Press.
Pekrun, R., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2014). International handbook of emotions in education. Routledge.
Pekrun, R., Murayama, K., Marsh, H. W., Goetz, T., & Frenzel, A. C. (2019). Happy fish in little ponds: Testing a reference group model of achievement and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(1), 166. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000230
Reilly, P. (2021). Promoting positive emotions among university EFL learners. Language Learning in Higher Education, 11(1), 153-173. https://doi.org/10.1515/cercles-2021-2013
Reilly, P., & Rosas, J. A. (2019). The achievement emotions of English language learners in Mexico. Electronic Journal of Foreign Language Teaching, 16(1), 34-48. https://e-flt.nus.edu.sg/v16n12019/reilly.pdf
Rezaee, A. A., & Oladrostam, E. (2018). Viewing IPPLL in light of gender differences: An exploratory approach. MEXTESOL Journal, 42(4). https://www.mextesol.net/journal/index.php?page=journal&id_article=4413
Richards, J. C. (2022). Exploring emotions in language teaching. RELC Journal, 53(1), 225-239.https://doi.org/10.1177/0033688220927531
Ringle, C. M., Wende, S., & Becker, J.-M. (2015). SmartPLS 3. Boenningstedt: SmartPLS. https://www.smartpls.com
Sadoughi, M., & Hejazi, S. Y. (2021). Teacher support and academic engagement among EFL learners: The role of positive academic emotions. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.stueduc.2021.101060
Sadoughi, M., & Hejazi, S. Y. (2022). Development and validation of Foreign Language Teacher Support Scale (FLTSS). Language Testing in Asia, 12. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40468-022-00186-1
Sadoughi, M., & Hejazi, S. Y. (2023a). How can L2 motivational self system enhance willingness to communicate? The contribution of foreign language enjoyment and anxiety. Current Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04479-3
Sadoughi, M., & Hejazi, S. Y. (2023b). The effect of teacher support on academic engagement: The serial mediation of learning experience and motivated learning behavior. Current Psychology, 42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03045-7
Schunk, D. H., & Greene, J. A. (2018). Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (2nd ed.). Routledge.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55(1), 5-14. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5
Shao, K., Nicholson, L. J., Kutuk, G., & Lei, F. (2020). Emotions and instructed language learning: Proposing a second language emotions and positive psychology model. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02142
Shao, K., Pekrun, R., & Nicholson, L. J. (2019). Emotions in classroom language learning: What can we learn from achievement emotion research? System, 86, 102-121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2019.102121
Swain, M. (2013). The inseparability of cognition and emotion in second language learning. Language Teaching, 46(2), 195-207. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0261444811000486
Tan, J.-M. (2017). Relationships between achievement emotions, motivation and language learning strategies of high, mid and low English language achievers. Journal of Literature and Art Studies, 7(6), 770-778. https://doi.org/10.17265/2159-5836/2017.06.014
Teimouri, Y. (2018). Differential roles of shame and guilt in L2 learning: How bad is bad? The Modern Language Journal, 102(4), 632-652. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12511
Teimouri, Y., Goetze, J., & Plonsky, L. (2019). Second language anxiety and achievement: A meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41(2), 363-387. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263118000311
Weiner, B. (2010). The development of an attribution-based theory of motivation: A history of ideas. Educational Psychologist, 45, 28–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520903433596
West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation models with nonnormal variables: Problems and remedies. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications (p. 56–75). Sage.
White, C. J. (2018). The emotional turn in applied linguistics and TESOL: Significance, challenges and prospects. In J. de D. Martínez Agudo (Ed.), Emotions in second language teaching: Theory, research and teacher education (pp. 19-34). Springer.
Yükselir, C., & Harputlu, L. (2014). An investigation into EFL prep-class students' academic emotions. Journal of Language and Linguistic Studies, 10(2), 100-119. https://www.jlls.org/index.php/jlls/article/view/236/198
Zare-ee, A., & Hejazi, Y. (2017). University teachers’ views on English as the medium of instruction in an Iranian higher education institution. Arab World English Journal (AWEJ), 8(4), 467-485. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3094548
Zare-ee, A., & Hejazi, S. Y. (2018). Iranian University Learners’ and Teachers’ Views on Adopted and Locally-Developed English Language Teaching Textbooks. International Journal of Instruction, 11(3), 291-308. https://doi.org/10.12973/iji.2018.11321a
Zhang, L., & Tsung, L. (2021). Learning Chinese as a second language in China: Positive emotions and enjoyment. System, 96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2020.102410
Zhang, X. (2019). Foreign language anxiety and foreign language performance: A meta‐analysis. The Modern Language Journal, 103(4), 763-781. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12590